Footage of 12 boys trapped in a cave system in Thailand inundated our screens in recent days.
An international rescue effort was under way, which included a team of specialists sent by the Australian government to assist with the safe recovery of the young soccer team. Highlighting the gravity of the situation, a former Thai Navy diver died after running out of oxygen during rescue efforts.
This was without doubt a frightening situation for the boys and their families. It's no surprise the situation received global media attention.
Though it does raise some interesting questions about how we extend empathy and concern to people we don't know.
Why did this tragedy capture the world's attention, when more long-term issues, such as children in detention, don't to the same extent?
Research from moral psychology can help us to understand this.
A picture is worth a thousand words
A key reason is simply that we could see the Thai soccer team. We watched the rescue effort play out, and we could see the emotions of the boys and their families.
We have seen this kind of viral, blanket coverage of other tragic incidents recently. For example, the horrific scenes of children fighting for their lives after the 2017 chemical weapon attacks in Syria.
Or the striking image that emerged in June of a small Honduran girl crying as her mother is detained by officials at the US-Mexico border.
A Honduran asylum seeker, 2, and her mother are taken into custody near the US-Mexico border. The Trump administration’s “zero tolerance” policy for undocumented immigrants calls for the separation of parents and children. #gettyimages #undocumented #gettyimagesnews pic.twitter.com/2tCmxDYlvd
— John Moore (@jbmoorephoto) June 13, 2018
By contrast, issues that are arguably no less frightening don't always generate the same outpouring of concern and sympathy.
For example, the more than 200 children held in detention on Nauru and throughout the Australian mainland.
This isn't to suggest the Australian government shouldn't aid in international rescue efforts, but we should be equally concerned about the far greater number of children being held indefinitely in Australian detention.
The fact is we have very little access to images of children in detention, as media access to Manus Island and Nauru is heavily restricted.
For example, journalists face substantial obstacles if they want to visit our offshore detention centres, and in 2016 the Australian government threatened health-care workers with jail time if they spoke about the conditions they encountered on Nauru and Manus.
We simply aren't permitted to view the plight of child refugees, and we're much less likely to experience an empathic response if we can't see them.
The recent outcry caused by the dramatic footage aboard an Australian live export ship illustrates this perfectly. Most of us would be aware to some extent live export is a cruel practice.
But it isn't until the footage forces us to confront the realities that we create enough momentum to discuss meaningful change.
The footage revealed a hell hole of overcrowding, a lack of food and water, and dead animals left to decay on the decks of the ship. #liveexportshttps://t.co/AyJ2bU1N3M
— RN Breakfast (@RNBreakfast) April 9, 2018
Time and perspective matters
The perspective we take also makes a huge difference. If we can easily draw comparisons between ourselves and those in need we're more likely to extend concern and empathy.
Given Australia's geography and climate, it's not too difficult for us to imagine our children caught up in a natural disaster.
It's much more difficult for us to imagine our children fleeing their homeland and seeking asylum in a foreign country.
And it's far easier to extend sympathy to a situation that, one way or another, will reach an end.
Ongoing humanitarian issues such as asylum seekers or food shortages on the African continent feel like immense challenges often placed in the too hard basket. Therefore, these issues fade away in the face of what we consider more pressing matters with more straightforward resolutions.
Language is crucial
The labels we attach are also crucial in determining our response.
For example, in 2016, former prime minister, Tony Abbott referred to asylum seekers as an invading force.
This sort of language is incredibly damaging, because when trying to make sense of a moral injustice we immediately look to identify both a victim and a villain. Suffering without a villain doesn't always make sense to us - though the villains we choose are often subjective.
There is some fascinating research demonstrating this. For example, throughout the US, belief in God is highest in states where citizens experience the greatest amount of suffering - infant mortality, cancer deaths and natural disasters.
This relationship holds after controlling for a range of alternative explanations, such as income and education. God is perceived to be the "villain" responsible for all this senseless suffering.
It's impossible to label those suffering at the hands of a chemical attack as anything but victims.
However, if we perceive asylum seekers as wrongdoers trying to steal some sort of unfair advantage, we're far less likely to think of them as victims requiring our compassion, meaning it's far easier to cast them out of our moral circle.
Do we have a moral responsibility to think differently?
Of course we should have sympathy for the soccer team that was trapped in the cave. But their story will disappear from our screens as the next pressing crisis arises.
We should ensure the reality of longer-term problems doesn't also disappear, having fallen victim to the failings of our moral cognition.
Science AF is ScienceAlert's new editorial section where we explore society's most complex problems using science, sanity and humour.
Dan Crimston, Postdoctoral Researcher in Morality and Social Psychology, The University of Queensland
This article was originally published on The Conversation. Read the original article.